Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Effectiveness of message framing on women’s intention to perform cytomegalovirus prevention behaviors: a cross-sectional study

Abstract

Background

The purpose of this study was to evaluate the effect of message framing on women’s intention to perform cytomegalovirus (CMV) prevention behaviors involving handwashing, not sharing food and eating utensils, not kissing a child on the lips and not placing a pacifier in the mouth after it was in a child’s mouth.

Methods

An online panel of women 18–40 years, who were pregnant or planning a pregnancy were randomized in a 2 × 2 factorial design to receive 1 of 4 CMV fact sheets. The fact sheets were framed as either what could be gained or be lost by following (or not) the recommendations and the likelihood of being affected by CMV (i.e., small chance or one of the most common infections in infants). The questionnaire measured CMV knowledge, participation in CMV risk or prevention behaviors, perceived severity of and susceptibly to CMV, and the perceived control over and the efficacy of recommended prevention behaviors. The dependent variable, intention to modify behavior, was an index score that ranged from 0 to 16 with higher values indicating greater intention. Linear regression was used to evaluate the association between all independent variables and overall behavioral intention.

Results

The sample included 840 women; 15.5% were familiar with CMV. Behavioral intention was high (M = 10.43; SD = 5.13) but did not differ across the message frames (p = 0.23). Overall, behavioral intention was predicted by CMV knowledge, message credibility, perceived severity of CMV, perceived behavioral control and response efficacy. Significant interactions with gain vs. loss frame were observed for perceived behavioral control (p = 0.03) and response efficacy (p = .003).

Conclusions

Framing CMV messages by what women stand to gain or lose interacts with perceived behavioral control and response efficacy to influence behavioral intention. Perceived behavioral control and response efficacy were most predictive of behavioral intention overall regardless of frame. Messaging that focuses on these two variables, particularly for avoiding kissing a child on the lips and sharing food, cups and utensils, may result in greater gains in intention to participate in CMV prevention behaviors.

Peer Review reports

Background

In the United States, congenital cytomegalovirus (CMV) infection is one of the most common causes of birth defects and developmental delays in infants. It is the leading cause of infant hearing loss [1] and can also result in cognitive and motor deficits, vision loss, or death [2]. Each year, there are nearly 26,000 U.S. children born with congenital CMV for a total birth prevalence of 0.64% [3]. Of these, approximately 400 infants will die and 8000 will develop lifelong disabilities [4]. Although the effects of this condition are serious, awareness of CMV among adults remains low at 13–39% [5,6,7].

Infants born with CMV are most commonly infected with the virus through transmission from mother to fetus during pregnancy [8]. The virus can also be transmitted through sexual contact, breastmilk, and organ transplantation [9]. Young children are the primary carriers of the virus [3]. If a woman is exposed to CMV for the first time prior to conception or at any time during pregnancy her infant may become infected [9]. Because no effective, licensed, CMV vaccine exists [10], behavioral practices are the only means for women to limit their exposure to CMV or reduce CMV transmission. Handwashing is currently the primary behavioral recommendation for reducing CMV transmission [11, 12]. However, research has shown that behaviors that limit a woman’s contact with children’s saliva may be especially efficacious in reducing infection [13]. These include avoiding contact with saliva when kissing a child and not putting things in one’s mouth that have been in a child’s mouth (specifically food, cups, forks or spoons, or pacifiers) [14]. Past research has shown that women regularly participate in behaviors that increase their risk of CMV infection through direct saliva sharing [15, 16]. Women also regularly engage in the protective behavior of handwashing [15, 16].

There is relatively limited research on how to effectively communicate CMV prevention information. Individual-level interventions, specifically healthcare provider counseling, have been shown to be effective in reducing CMV infection [17,18,19]. One study found that a fact sheet and video both encouraged women to practice individual prevention behaviors, look for more information about CMV, and share their knowledge with family members or friends [16]. There is no published research on the type of CMV message content that is most effective. Therefore, the purpose of this research was to explore the influence of message framing on women’s intentions to perform CMV prevention behaviors.

Message framing

Message framing refers to a common health communication technique in which behavior change messages are focused on the potential outcomes of a proposed health behavior. The theoretical basis for message framing stems from behavioral economic prospect theory [20]. Prospect theory’s premise is that when presenting a person with a choice between two alternatives, framing the choice in terms of the potential losses (cost of not complying) or gains (benefits) has differential effects on decision making and subsequent behavior. Gain and loss framed messages are structured around two components: 1) the outcomes or consequences of taking action or not taking action, and 2) the possibility that the specified outcome will be obtained, or not obtained [21]. Gain-framed messages focus on obtaining a desirable outcome (“If you quit smoking your lungs will be healthy and strong,”) or avoiding an undesirable outcome (“If you quit smoking you will be less likely to get lung cancer.”). Loss-framed messages emphasize being the recipient of the undesirable outcome (“If you do not quit smoking you may develop gum disease and lose your teeth,”) or failing to receive a desirable outcome by not following the advocated behavior (“If you do not quit smoking, you may not be able to keep your original teeth.”)

Message framing research suggests that the influence of gain and loss messages on advocated behaviors can be direct. In other words, the person can change behavior after hearing or seeing a message. Or it can be mediated by additional variables, one of which is behavioral intention [22]. Rogers [23] suggested that intention to perform a behavior results from an individual’s appraisal of three factors: severity of the event or outcome; perceived probability of the event occurring (susceptibility, or level of risk), and belief of whether following the recommended behavior will yield the desired result (response efficacy).

Additionally, research indicates that when a person perceives that individual risk is high, a loss frame tends to be more effective at influencing behavioral intention [24]; when risk perception is low people respond more positively to gain framed messages aimed at influencing behavioral intention [25] and actual behavior [26]. In some cases, among those who perceive they are not at risk, framing makes no difference in behavioral intention [24].

For response efficacy, studies have shown that loss frame is more effective at influencing behavioral intention for behaviors with low response efficacy [27, 28]. When there is high response efficacy, framing either has no effect on intention [27, 28] or gain frame messages are more persuasive [29,30,31]. Similarly, with high response efficacy, gain framed messages are more persuasive for actual behavior change [29].

In addition, the function of the proposed behavior can moderate the effect of message framing on intention and behavior [22]. Health behaviors are classified as having one of two functions, also known as roles: detecting potential health problems (e.g. mammography for breast cancer detection) or preventing future health issues (e.g. vaccination), also called protection behaviors [21]. Research on the interaction of behavioral function and message framing suggests that gain-framed messages may be more persuasive for protection-related behaviors [32, 33].

Given that CMV awareness is low, that there are significant consequences of CMV infection in infants, and that there is a scarcity of research in how to communicate this information, this study aimed to look at the effect of framed CMV messaging among women of childbearing years. Specifically we were interested in the association between message framing and behavioral intention for CMV prevention behaviors.

There were three hypotheses.

Hypothesis 1: Because the messages are focused on prevention-related behaviors, overall, gain-framed messages will result in greater intention to engage in CMV prevention behaviors.

Hypothesis 2: Messages that frame the potential risk or susceptibility of CMV infection as the most common infection will be associated with greater intention to engage in CMV prevention behaviors.

Hypothesis 3: For the group of women who perceive they are at risk for CMV infection, the loss frame will result in greater intention to engage in CMV prevention behaviors.

Method

Procedure and stimuli

This was a cross-sectional descriptive study using a 2 × 2 factorial design. Two likelihood conditions were chosen because the odds of being affected by CMV are low, yet CMV infection is one of the most common infections that can affect a baby [2]. A single outcome was framed as either what they would gain by practicing or lose by failing to practice the CMV prevention behaviors. This was chosen based on the message framing literature. The stimuli were four, one-page fact sheets about CMV which included research-supported behavioral recommendations from a previous study [16]. The text describing CMV and how it is spread was identical within each frame. Table 1 shows the key message content for each of the four frames.

Table 1 Critical content of the CMV fact sheets: Four framed messages

The fact sheets were pretested with a convenience sample of 113 women during four rounds of testing. Revisions were made to the fact sheets and messages for clarity and comprehension. The stimulus was embedded in a web-based survey and each respondent was randomized to receive one of the four fact sheets.

Sample

The sample included women aged 18–40 years who had a child 5 years of age or younger at home and were currently pregnant or planning to become pregnant within 12 months. The national United States sample was recruited during 2015 from an online panel managed by Qualtrics, a worldwide software research company. Women who had ever worked as a healthcare provider and those who had a child with a previously diagnosed disability were excluded as both groups were likely to have higher CMV awareness than the general population. Respondents were compensated in reward points credited to their Qualtrics account. The Brigham Young University institutional review board approved the study.

A sample size of 800 was selected so that we were able to have 200 in each cell (2 × 2 study design). The sample sizes in the message framing literature vary and average around 100 participants [27]. For our study, to estimate the proportion of women who know about CMV we needed a sample size of 384.

Two quality control measures were used during data collection. The first measure was a minimum time frame the respondent spent viewing the message. The second asked respondents to type in a specific word. Respondents who failed to meet the quality control criteria were excluded from the sample and were replaced until quotas were met.

Framing manipulation checks included four questions. These questions assessed whether the respondent had read and understood the fact sheet. The questions also measured whether survey respondents differentially perceived the messages in the four versions of the fact sheet.

Instrumentation

Outcome variable

The overall outcome variable was intention to perform the CMV prevention or risk behaviors. Behavioral intention was measured by one item for each of the eight behaviors. Respondents were asked how often they would engage in the behavior after reading the CMV fact sheet as compared to what they did before [15]. Responses were on a five point Likert scale from a lot less often to a lot more often. The optimal desired direction (either less or more) varied because to reduce risk, some behaviors should be less frequent (i.e. sharing food and utensils,) while others such as handwashing should be performed more often.

Predictor variables and constructs

The questionnaire assessed predictor variables selected based on past research with message framing studies: demographics, CMV awareness, message persuasiveness, and frequency of practicing CMV risk behaviors. Six constructs were also measured as detailed in Table 2 and in the following paragraphs. For each of the constructs an index score was created. For all scales, higher values indicated more of the construct. Demographic characteristics included education, household income, race/ethnicity, age of the youngest child at home, and pregnancy status.

Table 2 Psychometric Properties and Descriptive Statistics for Constructs Overall and by Message Frame

CMV awareness was assessed by asking respondents to rate their level of familiarity with CMV [16] and whether their healthcare provider had ever talked to them about CMV (yes or no). CMV background knowledge was measured by 12 items about CMV [16]. Response options were true (n = 6), false (n = 5) and I don’t know. All questions were coded 1 if correct and 0 if incorrect or don’t know. For each of the eight CMV prevention behaviors respondents were asked to rate on a 5-point Likert scale how often they performed or participated in each behavior [16]. Anchors for the three handwashing questions were never/always, while the scales for the remaining behaviors were ranged from never to every day.

Persuasiveness of the fact sheet material was assessed by asking respondents to indicate their agreement (strongly agree to strongly disagree) with one item, “If I were pregnant I would try to avoid catching CMV as a result of viewing this fact sheet” [16]. Message credibility was measured with three questions adapted from Regan et al. [34] which asked respondents to indicate their agreement that the fact sheet information was accurate, believable, and credible.

Perceived severity of CMV infection was measured with three questions adapted from Block and Keller [27] about the degree to which the respondent felt CMV infection in a baby was frightening, dangerous, or severe. Perceived susceptibility of CMV infection was measured by three items adapted from Nan [28] in which respondents indicated their level of agreement regarding the perceived likelihood, possibility of infection, and risk of getting CMV. Respondents answered one additional question about their perceived level of risk [35].

Perceived response efficacy of each of the CMV prevention behaviors was measured by one item adapted from Taber and Aspinwall [36]. The question asked how effective the respondent thought each behavior would be at decreasing their risk of getting CMV. CMV prevention behaviors, specifically those related to sharing with a child, inherently require the child’s cooperation. As such, respondents were asked to evaluate the extent to which they felt confident they could modify these behaviors (perceived behavioral control) and that it would be possible to do so [35].

Statistical analysis

Descriptive analysis

Frequencies, proportions, and scale means and spread were calculated to describe the sample’s sociodemographic characteristics, background knowledge, frequency of practicing CMV prevention or risk behaviors and the remaining constructs displayed in Table 2. The chi-square test was used to test for the difference in proportions across the frames and ANOVA was used to compare means across the frames. The framing condition was operationalized into two variables: 1) gain or loss frame and 2) small chance/most common.

Total score for overall behavioral intention

To measure the outcome variable, behavioral intention, the responses were re-coded and categorized as a three level variable with values of 0 (respondent behavior would remain the same), 1 (change behavior “slightly more/less” in the desired direction), and 2 (change behavior “a lot more/less” in the desired direction). Respondents who stated that they would change their behavior in the opposite of the desired direction (e.g., wash hands less; n = 23–43) were also classified as 0, no change in behavior. The 3-level items were then summed to create a behavioral intention index score ranging from 0 to 16 where higher values indicated greater intention. The resulting index score had a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.92.

Linear regression analysis- overall behavioral intention

Linear regression was used to evaluate the relationship of the independent variables on the overall behavioral intention scale. All variables were entered into the model as main effects and, as we were interested in testing potential moderating effect of framing conditions, all constructs were entered into the model as a two-way interaction with each of the two framing variables. To create a parsimonious model that adequately controlled for confounding, backward elimination was used to individually remove variables and interaction effects that did not achieve significance at the α = 0.10 level. Consistent with principles of hierarchically well-formulated models, non-significant main effects were retained if they were part of a significant interaction term.

Logistic regression analysis – Intention to change individual behaviors

To determine if the associations observed in the linear regression model varied across the CMV prevention behaviors, the 3-level behavioral intention index scores, were used as the dependent variable for multinomial logistic regression. The constructs and interactions that were significant in the linear regression model were included. Additionally, based on an a priori hypothesis that frequency of participation in behavior would affect behavioral intention, pre-survey frequency of performing the behavior was also included. All analyses were conducted in SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC, USA).

Results

There were 848 respondents. Of these, eight women incorrectly indicated that CMV could be caught from a mosquito and they were eliminated from further analysis for a final sample of 840. The mean age of women in the sample was 28.8 years (SD: 4.64). The majority of respondents (72.7%) were White, non-Hispanic and 40.4% reported having a college degree while 21.1% had a high school education or less. The plurality of women (32.5%) reported incomes in the range of $25,000 to less than $50,000. A quarter (25.6%) of women in the sample had income greater than $75,000 and 14% reported income <$25,000. Nearly half (47.5%) of the sample was currently pregnant and most women (67.0%) had a child 2 years or younger. Only 15.5% of women reported any familiarity with CMV and 6.1% had talked to a healthcare provider about CMV. There were no significant differences between the message frames with regard to demographics or CMV background knowledge.

Results indicate that the message framing conditions were manipulated effectively. There were statistically significant differences between the gain and loss message framing for both tone of the fact sheet and whether the fact sheet stressed the benefits of following the recommended behaviors (p < .001). The majority of respondents in each message frame correctly identified that there was a small chance of getting CMV (n = 326; 77.2%) or if CMV was one of the most common infections in newborn babies (n = 309; 73.9%).

The distribution of women in the highest risk behavioral groups for pre-survey participation in CMV risk and prevention behaviors is displayed in Fig. 1. CMV risk behaviors were common in the population with the exception of putting a pacifier in one’s mouth after it had been in a child’s mouth, which was reported at least weekly by only 25.71% of the sample. There were no differences across the four frames in the mean score for frequency of practicing each behavior (data not shown).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Percentage of respondents participating in CMV risk and prevention behaviors prior to the survey

The percentage of women who reported they would change each behavior is displayed in Fig. 2. Regardless of behavior, the majority of women indicated they would change their behavior after viewing the fact sheet. However, intention for kissing was lower as only 55.7% of women reported intention to decrease the frequency of kissing their child on the lips. The behavioral intention index score across all eight behaviors had a mean value of 10.43 (SD = 5.13).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Respondents reporting that they intended to change their behavior after viewing the CMV fact sheet. To prevent CMV, handwashing behaviors should increase, while all other behaviors should decrease

Message credibility and persuasiveness

The mean score for message credibility was not significantly different across the four message frames (Table 2). However, when examining the gain or loss framed messages separately, regardless if the message included the small chance or most common message, credibility scores were significantly different between gain frame (M = 5.65, SD = 1.08) and loss frame (M = 5.50, SD = 1.14; p = .03).

Women in the sample overwhelmingly stated they found the fact sheets persuasive. Nearly all respondents (93.9%; n = 789) reported they would try to avoid contracting CMV as a result of viewing the fact sheet. There were no statistically significant differences in persuasiveness across the frames (p = 0.32; data not shown).

Effects of message framing on behavioral intention

It was hypothesized that both the gain frame (hypothesis 1) and the most common frame (hypothesis 2) would be associated with increases in behavioral intention. However, no main effects on behavioral intention were observed with either the gain vs. loss or the small chance vs. most common message framing conditions. Two constructs had significant interactions with the gain vs. loss frame, specifically response efficacy and perceived behavioral control. No interaction effects with the small chance vs. most common frame achieved significance in the model. Additionally, perceived susceptibility and perceived level of risk failed to achieve significance in the model. The final regression model was adjusted for gain frame vs. loss frame, CMV knowledge, message credibility, perceived severity, perceived behavioral control and response efficacy along with the two interaction terms. The results of the linear regression are presented in Table 3. Overall, response efficacy and perceived behavioral control were most strongly associated with behavioral intention and appear to interact with gain and loss message framing.

Table 3 Multiple linear regression identifying factors associated with increased intention to modify CMV risk and prevention behaviors in the desired direction

Relationship between message frame and predictor variables and constructs

There were no significant differences between the four message frames for any of the variables or constructs (see Table 2). Similarly, no significant differences were observed on any of the six constructs when comparing small chance vs. most common irrespective of whether it included gain or loss frames (data not shown). When comparing the six constructs across gain and loss frames, regardless of small chance or most common frame, perceived susceptibility is marginally significant (p = 0.06) with those in the loss frame (M = 4.16, SD = 1.33) reporting higher mean levels of susceptibility than those in the gain frame (M = 3.98, SD = 1.38).

Predictors of behavioral intention

Perceived behavioral control and response efficacy were associated with the largest increases in overall behavioral intention (See Table 3). Given that significant interaction effects between gain and loss frame were observed for both behavioral control and response efficacy, these should be interpreted with consideration of framing. Regardless of frame, perceived behavioral control is associated with an increase in behavioral intention, however this effect is more pronounced among respondents in the gain frame. Conversely, the effect of response efficacy, although positively associated with behavioral intention in both frames, is more pronounced among those in the loss frame.

Recognizing that overall behavioral intention for reducing CMV risk requires a woman to engage in multiple behaviors and women may be more motivated to modify some than others, intention for each of the eight behaviors was examined individually. The logistic regression results for each of the eight individual CMV prevention behaviors are displayed in Tables 4, 5, and 6. Similar to the overall behavioral intention, perceived behavioral control and response efficacy were strongly associated with intention for individual behaviors. Although confidence intervals overlap for both handwashing and sharing behaviors, the association between perceived behavioral control and intention to change behavior “a lot” in the desired direction is stronger among those in the gain frame. Similarly, for both handwashing and sharing behaviors, the association between response efficacy and intention to change behavior “a lot” was stronger for the loss frame.

Table 4 Logistic regression predicting the odds of changing CMV handwashing prevention behavior in the desired direction
Table 5 Logistic regression predicting the odds of changing CMV sharing prevention behaviors in the desired direction
Table 6 Logistic regression predicting the odds of changing CMV kissing on the lips and pacifier use prevention behaviors in the desired direction

The effect of loss frame messaging among women who perceived they were at risk

It was hypothesized that among those who see themselves at risk for CMV infection, the loss frame would be associated with increased behavioral intention (Hypothesis 3). Among the subset of women who agreed or strongly agreed that they were at risk for CMV infection (n = 471), there was no association between framing and overall behavioral intention (p = .20) in the unadjusted model or the adjusted model (p = 0.70). However, among this same group of women, the mean susceptibility score was higher for those in the most common frame (M = 5.01; SD = 0.83) compared to the small chance frame (M = 4.82; SD = 0.89; p = 0.02). Similarly, the most common frame was also associated with higher levels of perceived risk with a mean level of 2.74 (SD = 1.01) compared to 2.50 (SD = 1.17) in the small chance frame (p = 0.02). There were no differences in mean risk level between overall loss frames (M = 2.64; SD = 1.15) and gain frames (M = 2.60; SD = 1.05).

Discussion

This study was undertaken to explore the effectiveness of message framing in communicating with women of child-bearing age about how to reduce CMV infection. There were no differences among sample demographics for the four message frames, therefore any difference between the groups with regard to behavioral intention or other constructs should have been due to the messaging. Results showed that message framing had no direct effect on overall behavioral intention. However, there were main effects for knowledge, message credibility, perceived severity, response efficacy and perceived behavioral control. Additionally, interactions with gain and loss frame were noted for behavioral control and response efficacy with associated increases in behavioral intention.

Consistent with other CMV research, we observed high rates of participation in CMV risk behaviors, particularly for sharing food, utensils and cups with children and kissing a child on the lips [15, 16]. Although these behaviors were common, behavioral intention was generally high, indicating that women were willing to modify these behaviors to protect their unborn child. It may be that because CMV awareness was relatively low, exposure to any information regardless of whether it was presented as gains or losses resulted in increased behavioral intention.

The lack of between-group differences for behavioral intention is similar to the results in a meta-analysis on prevention behavior-related messages which observed no difference between gain and loss framing in persuasiveness [37]. Likewise, other studies have found no effect with gain and loss messages regarding vaccination behaviors and intentions to perform behaviors [38, 39]. O’Keefe and colleagues suggested that gain and loss framed messages may not be effective for prevention-related behaviors because outcomes are not certain [37]. This may be true for CMV messaging as well. Although studies have shown that the prevention behaviors included in the fact sheet can reduce CMV infection [14], there are no data that indicate the certainty or magnitude of risk reduction that may occur as a result of doing those behaviors. The absence of between-group differences may also be due to the possibility that subtle messaging differences about losses and gains could have been overshadowed by the CMV information which would have been new to the majority of respondents.

Response efficacy, perceived behavioral control, perceived severity of a CMV infection, message credibility and CMV knowledge were all associated with increases in behavioral intention, though at varying levels. However, it appears that a woman’s perception of effectiveness of the behavior to reduce risk is most influential in her behavioral intention. In other research, a woman’s perception about the effectiveness of the flu vaccine influenced whether or not she received the vaccine [38]. In our study, the perception of effectiveness varied across behaviors with respondents feeling that hand hygiene was most effective at reducing risk of CMV infection. Not sharing food or utensils were seen as less effective than handwashing. Half of the respondents felt that not kissing a child on the lips was very effective (data not shown). Interestingly, the fact sheet did not mention the effectiveness of performing the CMV prevention behaviors, suggesting that people have predetermined beliefs about these behaviors that influence perceptions of efficacy. This may indicate that response efficacy is a function of other constructs that if identified may be modifiable.

The observed interactions that varied between gain frame and perceived behavioral control, and loss frame and response efficacy, may indicate that framing has the potential to differentially impact some subsets of women. A study about sun protection behaviors also observed an interaction between framing and response efficacy for one of three sun protection behaviors [31]. However, loss frame was more persuasive among those with low response efficacy and gain frame was more persuasive for those with high response efficacy [31]. This differs from our finding that behavioral intention increased with increasing levels of response efficacy, but the increase was more pronounced for those in the loss frame. It is unclear whether the differences between these two studies reflect differences in populations or the behaviors studied, or some other factor that differs between CMV prevention and sun protection behaviors.

Although behavioral intention scores were in the positive direction, intention was not equal across all behaviors. A smaller percentage of women reported intention to change sharing behaviors or kissing a child on the lips compared with washing hands. Possible explanations for the relative unwillingness to modify these behaviors (sharing and kissing a child on the lips) may be because these require the cooperation of others and are rooted in cultural norms which could have significant mental and emotional costs if changed. For example, parents often share cups, utensils and food with their child for convenience or to model appropriate eating practices [40]. Kissing a child on the lips is a very common and valued expression of affection [41]. Yet, these are two high-risk behaviors because of the potential sharing of saliva that have high CMV viral loads [3]. Though potentially difficult to change, there have been shifts in cultural norms related to other maternal-child health behaviors such as putting a child to sleep on his/her back [42] and using car safety seats [43].

Additional research could explore how framing interacts with predictors of behavioral intention in various populations and across various types of CMV prevention behaviors. Nevertheless, it is important to recognize that regardless of frame, increased response efficacy, behavioral control, and perceptions of severity were associated with increases in behavioral intention. This suggests that although the complex interaction between framing and these constructs is not fully understood, messaging focused on these constructs may influence behavioral intention. Additionally, recent research on message framing suggests that personal motivating factors such as preferences for self-regulation (e.g., pleasure vs. pain or rewards vs. punishments) may moderate message framing effects [44,45,46]. This is an unexplored area in CMV research.

Further, the differential impact of messaging on actual behavior change is not known. Additional areas of inquiry should include the testing of CMV messaging strategies in a sample of women who are aware of CMV in order to determine if any information about CMV versus tailored CMV messages influence intention and change. Furthermore, longitudinal research to determine the relationships between behavioral intention and realized behavioral change would be useful.

Limitations

The study measured only perceived behavioral intention to follow guidelines and not actual behavior. There is no indication that intention to do CMV prevention behaviors will lead to behavior change. However, numerous research studies have demonstrated that intentions are good predictors of actual behavior [47, 48]. The data are self-reported and women may have over- or understated what they do, though the rates of behavior are similar to other CMV studies [15, 16]. Intention to change behavior was measured immediately after seeing the fact sheet and may have suffered from a ceiling effect. There was no control group with which to make comparisons.

Our demographic distribution was similar to other national survey panels studying CMV [15]. The study sample was primarily white with at least some college education. The percent of women with a college degree or greater is higher than the US estimate of 30% [49]. The median US household income is $53,000 (US) [50] and though we cannot directly compare with this study’s income categories, the distribution indicates that our sample maybe be more wealthy. CMV seroprevalence rates are highest among groups with lower socioeconomic status and certain racial-ethnic minority groups [51]. CMV awareness also varies by demographic variables [15]. This data may not be reflective of cultural norms and practices of all racial and ethnic groups or may not represent groups that could benefit the most from CMV messaging. It is possible that these panel women may not represent all women who are pregnant or thinking about becoming pregnant.

Conclusion

Congenital CMV infection is common and can lead to negative health outcomes. Because awareness is low, and severity is high, determining how to construct CMV prevention messages for optimal influence is of utmost importance. Framing messages by what women have to gain or lose by participating in CMV prevention behaviors does not make a difference in overall behavioral intention. In this study, perceived behavioral control and response efficacy were most predictive of behavioral intention. Additionally, framing interacts with perceived effectiveness of CMV prevention behaviors and individual perceptions of behavioral control to influence intention. There may be other factors that are more persuasive when trying to influence women’s behaviors that may reduce the sharing of a young child’s saliva which has high CMV viral loads. Future messaging that focuses on increasing perceived behavioral control and response efficacy, particularly for the kissing and sharing behaviors, may result in greater gains in intention.

Abbreviations

CMV:

Cytomegalovirus

References

  1. Grosse SD, Ross DS, Dollard SC. Congenital cytomegalovirus (CMV) infection as a cause of permanent bilateral hearing loss: a quantitative assessment. J Clin Virol. 2008;41(2):57–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Dollard SC, Grosse SD, Ross DS. New estimates of the prevalence of neurological and sensory sequelae and mortality associated with congenital cytomegalovirus infection. Rev Med Virol. 2007;17(5):355–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Cannon MJ, Griffiths PD, Aston V, Rawlinson WD. Universal newborn screening for congenital CMV infection: what is the evidence of potential benefit? Rev Med Virol. 2014;24(5):291–307.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Cannon MJ, Davis KF. Washing our hands of the congenital cytomegalovirus disease epidemic. BMC Public Health. 2005;5:70. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-5-70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Jeon J, Victor M, Adler SP, Arwady A, Demmler G, Fowler K, Goldfarb J, Keyserling H, Massoudi M, Richards K, Staras SAS, Cannon MJ. Knowledge and awareness of congenital cytomegalovirus among women. Infectious Disease in Obstetrics and Gynecology 2006; Article ID 80383, 7 pages. doi:10.1155/IDOG/2006/80383

  6. Pereboom MT, Mannien J, Spelten ER, Schellevis FG, Hutton EK. Observational study to assess pregnant women's knowledge and behaviour to prevent toxoplasmosis, listeriosis and cytomegalovirus. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2013;13:98. doi:10.1186/1471-2393-13-98.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Willame A, Blanchard-Rohner G, Combescure C, Irion O, Posfay-Barbe K, Martinez de Tejada B. Awareness of cytomegalovirus infection among pregnant women in Geneva, Switzerland: a cross-sectional study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2015;12(12):15285–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Schleiss MR. Congenital cytomegalovirus infection: update on management strategies. Curr Treat Options Neurol. 2008;10(3):186–92.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Boppana SB. Cytomegalovirus. In: Hutto C, editor. Congenital and perinatal infections: a concise guide to diagnosis. Totowa, New Jersey: Humana Press; 2006. p. 73–86.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  10. Plotkin S. The history of vaccination against cytomegalovirus. Med Microbiol Immunol (Berl). 2015;204(3):247–54.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. American Academy of Pediatrics. Cytomegalovirus infection. In: Kimberlin DW, Brady MT, Jackson MA, Long SS, editors. Red book. 2015 report of the committee on infectious diseases. 30th ed. Elk Grove: IL: American Academy of Pediatrics; 2015. p. 317–22.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention: Cytomegalovirus (CMV) and Congenital CMV Infection. http://www.cdc.gov/cmv/overview.html. Accessed 16 September 2016.

  13. Cannon MJ, Stowell JD, Clark R, Dollard PR, Johnson D, Mask K, Stover C, Wu K, Amin M, Hendley W, Guo J. Repeated measures study of weekly and daily cytomegalovirus shedding patterns in saliva and urine of healthy cytomegalovirus-seropositive children. BMC Infect Dis. 2014 Nov 13;14(1):569.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Harrison GJ. Current controversies in diagnosis, management, and prevention of congenital cytomegalovirus: updates for the pediatric practitioner. Pediatr Ann. 2015;44(5):e115–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Cannon MJ, Westbrook K, Levis D, Schleiss MR, Thackeray R, Pass RF. Awareness of and behaviors related to child-to-mother transmission of cytomegalovirus. Prev Med. 2012;54(5):351–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Price SM, Bonilla E, Zador P, Levis DM, Kilgo CL, Cannon MJ. Educating women about congenital cytomegalovirus: assessment of health education materials through a web-based survey. BMC Womens Health. 2014;14(1):144. doi:10.1186/s12905-014-0144-3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Adler SP, Finney JW, Manganello M, Best AM. Prevention of child-to-mother transmission of cytomegalovirus among pregnant women. J Pediatr. 2004;145:485–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Revello MG, Tibaldi C, Masuelli G, Frisina V, Sacchi A, Furione M, Arossa A, Spinillo A, Klersy C, Ceccarelli M. Prevention of primary cytomegalovirus infection in pregnancy. EBioMedicine. 2015;2(9):1205–10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Vauloup-Fellous C, Picone O, Cordier A, Parent-du-Châtelet I, Senat M, Frydman R, Grangeot-Keros L. Does hygiene counseling have an impact on the rate of CMV primary infection during pregnancy?: Results of a 3-year prospective study in a French hospital. J Clin Virol. 2009;46(Supplement 4):S49–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Tversky A, Kahneman D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science. 1981;211(4481):453–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Rothman AJ, Salovey P. Shaping perceptions to motivate healthy behavior: the role of message framing. Psychol Bull. 1997;121(1):3–19.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Updegraff JA, Rothman AJ. Health message framing: moderators, mediators, and mysteries. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2013;7(9):668–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Rogers RWA. Protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change. Aust J Psychol. 1975;91(1):93–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Gerend MA, Shepherd JE. Using message framing to promote acceptance of the human papillomavirus vaccine. Health Psychol. 2007;26(6):745–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Hull SJ. Perceived risk as a moderator of the effectiveness of framed HIV-test promotion messages among women: a randomized controlled trial. Health Psychol. 2012;31(1):114–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Updegraff JA, Brick C, Emanuel AS, Mintzer RE, Sherman DK. Message framing for health: moderation by perceived susceptibility and motivational orientation in a diverse sample of Americans. Health Psychol. 2015;34(1):20–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Block LG, Keller PA. When to accentuate the negative: the effects of perceived efficacy and message framing on intentions to perform a health-related behavior. J Mark Res. 1995;32(2):192–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Nan X, Xie B, Madden K. Acceptability of the H1N1 vaccine among older adults: the interplay of message framing and perceived vaccine safety and efficacy. Health Commun. 2012;27(6):559–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Apanovitch AM, McCarthy D, Salovey P. Using message framing to motivate HIV testing among low-income, ethnic minority women. Health Psychol. 2003;22(1):60–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Bartels RD, Kelly KM, Rothman AJ. Moving beyond the function of the health behaviour: the effect of message frame on behavioural decision-making. Psychol Health. 2010;25(7):821–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Hwang Y, Cho H, Sands L, Jeong SH. Effects of gain- and loss-framed messages on the sun safety behavior of adolescents: the moderating role of risk perceptions. J Health Psychol. 2012;17(6):929–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Gallagher KM, Updegraff JA. Health message framing effects on attitudes, intentions, and behavior: a meta-analytic review. Ann Behav Med. 2012;43(1):101–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Rothman AJ, Bartels RD, Wlaschin J, Salovey P. The strategic use of gain-and loss-framed messages to promote healthy behavior: how theory can inform practice. Aust J Commun. 2006;56(s1):S202–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Regan Á, McConnon Á, Kuttschreuter M, Rutsaert P, Shan L, Pieniak Z, Barnett J, Verbeke W, Wall P. The impact of communicating conflicting risk and benefit messages: an experimental study on red meat information. Food Qual Prefer. 2014;38:107–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Yardley L, Miller S, Schlotz W, Little P. Evaluation of a web-based intervention to promote hand hygiene: exploratory randomized controlled trial. J Med Internet Res. 2011;13(4):e107. doi:10.2196/jmir.1963.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Taber JM, Aspinwall LG. Framing recommendations to promote prevention behaviors among people at high risk: a simulation study of responses to melanoma genetic test reporting. J Genet Couns. 2015;24(5):1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. O'Keefe DJ, Jensen JD. The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed loss-framed messages for encouraging disease prevention behaviors: a meta-analytic review. J Health Commun. 2007;12(7):623–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Frew PM, Saint-Victor DS, Owens LE, Omer SB. Socioecological and message framing factors influencing maternal influenza immunization among minority women. Vaccine. 2014;32(15):1736–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Gainforth HL, Cao W, Latimer-Cheung AE. Message framing and parents' intentions to have their children vaccinated against HPV. Public Health Nurs. 2012;29(6):542–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Patrick H, Nicklas TAA. Review of family and social determinants of children’s eating patterns and diet quality. J Am Coll Nutr. 2005;24(2):83–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Johnson TC, Hooper RI. Boundaries and family practices: implications for assessing child abuse. J child. Sex Abus. 2004;12(3–4):103–25.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Adams SM, Ward CE, Garcia KL. Sudden infant death syndrome. Am Fam Physician. 2015;91(11):778–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Winston FK, Chen IG, Elliott MR, Arbogast KB, Durbin DR. Recent trends in child restraint practices in the United States. Pediatrics. 2004;113(5):e458–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Covey J. The role of dispositional factors in moderating message framing effects. Health Psychol. 2014;33(1):52–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Cesario J, Corker KS, Jelinek SA. Self-regulatory framework for message framing. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2013;49(2):238–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Van’t Riet J, Cox AD, Cox D, Zimet GD, De Bruijn G, Van den Putte B, De Vries H, Werrij MQ, Ruiter RA. Does perceived risk influence the effects of message framing? A new investigation of a widely held notion. Psychol Health. 2014;29(8):933–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Sheeran P. Intention—behavior relations: a conceptual and empirical review. Eur Rev Soc Psychol. 2002;12(1):1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Webb TL, Sheeran P. Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol Bull. 2006;132(2):249–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. US Census Bureau. American Fact Finder. Sex by educational attainment for the population 25 years and over. Retrieved from US Census Bureau website. http://factfinder.census.gov/faces/tableservices/jsf/pages/productview.xhtml?pid=ACS_14_1YR_B15002&prodType=table. Updated 2014. Accessed 26 Aug 2016.

  50. US Census Bureau. Quick Facts. United States. Retrieved from US Census Bureau website. https://www.census.gov/quickfacts/table/PST045215/00. Updated 2015. Accessed 26 Aug 2016.

  51. Bate SL, Dollard SC, Cannon MJ. Cytomegalovirus seroprevalence in the United States: the National Health and nutrition examination surveys, 1988-2004. Clin Infect Dis. 2010;50(11):1439–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

None.

Funding

Funding was provided from internal university funds. The funding body had no role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript.

Availability of data and materials

Data upon which the analysis is based is available upon reasonable request from the authors. Institutional review board approval did not grant permission to post data publicly.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RT designed the study, coordinated data collection, and prepared the manuscript. BMM designed the study, conducted data analysis, and prepared the manuscript. EMC assisted with study design and preparing the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rosemary Thackeray.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the Brigham Young University institutional review board and assigned study number 15240. A consent document was provided on the screen when respondents began the on-line survey; consent was implied when the participant completed the survey.

Consent for publication

Not applicable

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Thackeray, R., Magnusson, B.M. & Christensen, E.M. Effectiveness of message framing on women’s intention to perform cytomegalovirus prevention behaviors: a cross-sectional study. BMC Women's Health 17, 134 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12905-017-0492-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12905-017-0492-x

Keywords